Digilitey.eu

Picture Storybooks Go Digital:
Pros and Cons
Daisy Smeets & Adriana Bus
an increasing number of picture storybooks for young children have become available electronically, and there has been an explosion in children's storybook apps, especially since tablet computers such as iPads have become popular. Today's e-books have evolved significantly from platforms that once only displayed simple digitized versions of print books to tools that can now support highly interactive, multimedia experiences. At a minimum, multimedia additions to electronic picture storybooks include at least an oral reading of the story text. In addition, they may involve symbolic elements typically not used with print books: live-action video, digital graphics, animations, music, sound effects, and interactivity of text (e.g., highlighting of text while it is narrated). Some educators are of the opinion that the multimedia additions are unnecessary and may even work counterproductively. Interaction with adults is still considered to be the optimal way to familiarize children with stories and the varied and rich vocabulary in picture storybooks. On the other hand, additional features may provide new opportunities for text comprehension.
In the age of Common Core State Standards (National Governors Association Center for Best Practices & Council of Chief State School Officers, 2010), teachers may wonder whether e-books are appropriate tools for familiarizing 4- to 6-year-olds with children's literature and contributing to a positive relationship with reading that lasts a lifetime: When young children have learned to enjoy books, they will continue to read, whereby technical reading and oral language skills keep improving, resulting in sustained motivation to read (Mol & Bus, 2011). The gold standard of book reading to 4- to 6-year-olds is interactive reading that combines an oral rendition of text with clarification about the story text through the actions of pointing at pictures and highlighting details mentioned in the text (Whitehurst & Lonigan, 2001). In addition to building comprehension skills, shared book reading may also promote print knowledge, especially when parents actively focus children's Quality Reading Instruction in the Age of Common Core Standards, edited by Susan B. Neuman and Linda B. Gambrell. 2013 by the International Reading Association.
attention on print features (e.g., Justice & Ezell, 2000). Therefore, teachers of preschoolers and kindergarten children may want to explore whether additional multimedia features can bolster similar or additional beneficial effects of e-book reading. As the number of living storybooks available on the Internet and through apps and other digital media increases, it becomes more and more imperative to examine effects of electronic books that allow young children to hear a story and explore a range of activities while the tale moves along.
Shared Book Reading
One of the first studies of the Leiden group appeared to give interactive,
multimedia books on the computer the green light (de Jong & Bus, 2004).
The study compared the gold standard of shared print book reading with
independent reading of interactive books on the computer. In the shared
reading condition, the adult stimulated children to make inferences (e.g.,
"Do you see why the bride cannot sit on the luggage carrier?") or to make
a connection between the text and picture (e.g., paraphrasing the text the
experimenter pointed to the picture, saying, "See, Little Tiger is carried
first by Little Bear, and then by the strong wolf and the strong goat"). In
the independent e-book condition, children "read" a story from the same
book series on their own. The digitized Janosch books (1998a, 1998b)
included, apart from an oral rendition of the tale, click-accessible hotspots
that revealed a sound or animation, mostly inconsistent with the story
text (e.g., a tea towel turns into a dove). With simple filmic means, the
illustrations in the book were animated to dramatize events highlighted in
the story text (e.g., the doctor bends toward the X-ray saying: "Ah, a faulty
stripe."). Other depictions in the illustration that were not highlighted in
the text were not animated.
Children's retellings of the story text were of the same quality whether children shared the book with an adult who actively involved the children by asking questions (the gold standard of reading) or "read" the animated, interactive computer version on their own. There was no evidence that the added eye and ear candy (the hotspots revealing effects that were surprising but mostly not helpful for understanding the story text) distracted children to the detriment of story comprehension. Apparently, for parents or teachers looking for viable options for children to listen to stories, e-books can meet the need without placing a heavy burden on the adult. Researchers from the Joan Ganz Cooney Center (Chiong, Ree, Takeuchi, & Erickson, 2012) even conclude that attempts by adults to share interactive e-books with their child may distort the e-book reading Picture Storybooks Go Digital: Pros and Cons 177 experience. Interactive e-books are mostly designed in a way that does not allow adults to access and control settings to customize a shared reading.
The researchers asked 32 pairs of parents and their 3- to 6-year-old children to share both a print book and an e-book. Half of the pairs read a basic e-book (where pictures appear on the computer but no multimedia features are included), and the other half read an enhanced e-book (which included highly interactive, multimedia experiences). The results buttressed the argument that it is preferable to choose print or basic e-books when you read to children and want to prioritize literacy-building experiences over ones intended "just for fun." The enhanced e-book was much less effective than a print and basic e-book in supporting the benefits of shared reading, probably because the e-book enhancements in the target book were not designed in a way that allows parents to customize interactive experiences. Apps included fun animations that may easily provoke non-content related actions (e.g., behavior- or device-focused talk, pushing hands away). In sum, the extra features of enhanced e-books appeared to distract adults and children alike from the story, negatively affecting the nature of conversation and the amount of detail children recall.
Bells and Whistles in Enhanced E-Books
Unlike traditional printed picture storybooks, electronic storybooks show
less homogeneity in design features due to a variety of bells and whistles
(de Jong & Bus, 2003; Korat & Shamir, 2004; Roskos, Brueck, & Widman,
2009). In some books, pictures are similar to those in the printed version of
the book, but other books may include animated (details of) pictures. Apart
from an oral rendition of text, electronic picture storybooks may include
sound effects and background music. Furthermore, some electronic
stories include games or other interactive features such as hotspots, often
revealing some kind of audio or visual effect when clicked on. How do
these e-book features affect children's language and literacy development?
Animated Illustrations
The Leiden group found beneficial effects of e-books that presented
animated illustrations instead of static ones in addition to an oral rendition
of text (e.g., Smeets & Bus, 2012b; Verhallen & Bus, 2010; Verhallen, Bus,
& de Jong, 2006). In these studies, first- and second-language learners
repeatedly heard either a storybook with animated illustrations that
dramatize the story, as in the Janosch series (e.g., 1998a, 1998b), or an
electronic version of a similar story with merely static pictures. Text
178 Smeets & Bus comprehension—especially vocabulary—improved when children "read" the animated e-book. We assume that the animated parts of pictures make it easier for children to match relevant details in illustrations with oral text, thus facilitating word and text comprehension. Animated details attract more visual attention than other parts, which may promote contiguity between oral text and illustration; that is, corresponding text is pronounced simultaneously with animated details in the illustration. For example, the text "Winnie is furious" in the story Winnie the Witch (Thomas & Gorky, 1996) is accompanied by an illustration showing an angry-looking witch making wild movements, which may help to coordinate the text with Winnie's behavior. Likewise, camera effects, such as zoom, may help to match relevant details in illustrations with oral text, thereby supporting children's text comprehension.
Normally, children fixate more than 90% of their time on the illustrations in picture storybooks while they listen to an oral reading of text (e.g., Evans & Saint-Aubin, 2005) and, what probably is most important, they fixate more often and longer on details in illustrations that are highlighted in the story text than on elements that are not (Verhallen & Bus, 2011). That is, while looking at illustrations in books, children search for elements that are highlighted in the story text. For example, while listening to a story about a mouse who wants to pick strawberries and therefore brought his wheelbarrow with him, we see their eyes successively fixate on the mouse, the strawberries, and the wheelbarrow. Lavishly illustrated storybooks for young children may complicate children's finding their way through the illustration and may instead coordinate what children look at in illustrations with the oral text. Take, for instance, the storybook Rokko the Crocodile (de Wijs, 2001). One of the illustrations shows Rokko and his parents watching a dozen crocodile eggs, waiting for Rokko's siblings to appear. The illustration shows, in addition to intact eggs, some eggs that are broken. Sometimes a baby crocodile's snout is visible, which demonstrates how young crocodiles appear from an egg. However, other broken eggs show a tail, which could also mean that a crocodile is crawling inside the egg. By and large, there is much room for errors in interpreting the story language (e.g., what is meant by siblings and what does appearing mean?).
Simultaneous processing of story text and illustrations, as is facilitated by animated e-books (Schnotz & Rasch, 2005), may explain why animated versions of picture storybooks were preferable to versions with only static pictures. Holding verbal and visual information in working memory at the same time may help children build stronger connections between visual and verbal information. When a word is processed through both the verbal Picture Storybooks Go Digital: Pros and Cons 179 and visual channels, this stimulates dual coding of information (Paivio, 1986), which facilitates storing and retaining word meanings. It seems evident that the quality of animations is vital. Film-like presentations of stories may not have the same effects when they include "wild" animations that do not match the story text. The "crowdedness" of animated presentations, requiring children to process simultaneously through multiple modalities that do not match, may even cause overload of the brain, which may reduce learning about the story language. We expect, therefore, negative results for cartoon-like e-books that include numerous animations that do not match the story language.
Sound Effects and Music
Likewise, additional audio features in electronic books are perceptually
salient and thus likely to elicit and sustain children's attention (e.g., Barr,
Zack, Garcia, & Muentener, 2008). Moreover, background music may lend
support to story comprehension, for instance by stressing the characters'
moods (e.g., sadness or happiness). Adding the sound of knocking on a
door, birds whistling, or a motor humming may help clarify these words'
meanings. When sounds are coded simultaneously with the oral text, dual
coding may facilitate storage and retrieval of unknown words (Paivio, 1986).
A recent study (Smeets, van Dijken, & Bus, 2012) shows that background music and sounds are not beneficial for all children, however. When children are diagnosed with severe language impairments (SLI), aural multimedia additions distort the beneficial effects of video stories. Instead of supporting text comprehension by emphasizing suspense, stressing moods (such as happiness or sadness), and clarifying the meanings of words, the addition of music and sounds seems to complicate the perception of speech by these children. Differential effects of video and music as this study revealed for SLI children have not been examined in normal populations, but it may be worth the effort to find out which elements of video storybooks make these e-books more or less effective in other groups (for instance in easily distractible groups). The finding that music and sounds were detrimental for word learning in children with SLI has far-reaching implications for e-book development. In many storybook apps, the volume of the soundtrack is loud and not adjustable, which may be a problem for children with language delays in particular. In Disney's Cars 2, the narration can barely be heard with the exciting background music and sound effects of race cars driving by. The storybook app Magic Gold Fish by Yasmin Studios includes hotspots with sound effects (e.g., a fish splashing in the water) that are very loud 180 Smeets & Bus in comparison with the narration. Interestingly, the app Pansjo Tummy developed by ThenQ includes an option to turn off background music and narration separately; such a feature makes an e-book more adaptive to support learning in different target groups.
Interactivity in Enhanced E-Books
Games—Too Many Choices?
Largely due to the increased popularity of touch screen devices, a
considerable quantity of e-books includes interactive features such
as puzzles, coloring, or memory games, but there is concern for the
educational quality of these stories (e.g., de Jong & Bus, 2003; Korat &
Shamir, 2004; Roskos et al., 2009; Zucker, Moody, & McKenna, 2009). These
activities might be entertaining and increase children's motivation (e.g.,
Ricci & Beal, 2002), but they may also promote a "game-playing mode"
rather than stimulate children to focus on the story (de Jong & Bus, 2002;
Labbo & Kuhn, 2000).
A study targeting one of the first digitized picture storybooks that appeared on the Dutch market, P.B. Bear's Birthday Party (Davis, 1996), showed that too many choices of interactive options is risky given that most young children bring a game-playing mentality to the computer (personal communication with Linda Labbo, April 19, 2004). Each screen of P.B. Bear's Birthday Party offered a choice of hotspots to click on resulting in one or more of the following: • Speech feedback for the complete text on the screen or speech feedback for single-text phrases or individual words • A film-like presentation of the events (e.g., P.B. Bear starts unwrapping the package as described by the text) • Animations (clicking on rebus-like icons that replace the nouns in the printed text, the written word appears simultaneously with short, 4.5-second animations and their pronunciation) As none of the actions happened automatically, the program opened up the possibility to revert to game playing without paying attention to the story text. The result, as concluded by de Jong and Bus (2002), was that the iconic modes of this book (e.g., animated illustrations, animations, games) attracted the 4- and 5-year-old participants' attention at the expense of listening to the oral story text. After six 15-minute sessions, only a minority of the children in this study had listened to the entire story text more than once, even though they had sufficient opportunity to Picture Storybooks Go Digital: Pros and Cons 181 read the story six times. Actually, children explored P.B. Bear's Birthday Party in bits and pieces, activating animations, accessing text fragments, and ignoring connected text. As a result, the children in the experimental condition did not know the story content better than those in a control condition, who played some other computer game and did not hear the story about P.B. Bear. A game-playing disposition may dominate with such books unless teachers carefully establish what students are expected to do with their time with e-books.
Hotspots
In general it is assumed that interactivity is especially effective due to
children's active involvement in extracting meanings (Sénéchal, 1997;
Sénéchal et al., 1995). Television shows for young children that promote
active involvement have been demonstrated to support learning more than
programs without this feature (e.g., Crawley, Anderson, Wilder, Williams,
& Santomero, 1999). Nickelodeon's Dora the Explorer and Blue's Clues
e-books include, therefore, a "tutor" who makes eye contact with children,
asks them questions, pauses for the child to respond, and reacts to the
child's answer. Likewise, the first generation of enhanced e-books included
hotspots that elaborated on story events or on words in the text. For
instance, the story ‘I'll Make You Well Again,' Said the Bear (Janosch, 1998b)
contained a scene where Tiger is given an injection prior to an operation.
A click on the needle triggers a visualization of how the fluid spreads out
in the body. Exploring more recent e-books, we came across many more
examples of hotspots that support comprehension of text and story events.
In the Dr. Seuss series developed by Oceanhouse Media and Dr. Seuss
Enterprises (e.g., A Cat in the Hat), objects are defined when clicked on,
similar to how parents or teachers may explain the meanings of difficult
words (e.g., Biemiller & Boote, 2006; Dickinson & Smith, 1994; Elley, 1989).
Korat and colleagues have reported that e-books with a built-in option to receive word definitions on demand promote word learning (e.g., Shamir & Korat, 2009) in both typically developing children (Shamir, Korat, & Shlafer, 2011) and children at risk for language delays (Shamir et al., 2011). The studies reveal significant pre- to posttest gains in word learning but do not offer concrete evidence for the effects of interactive word-meaning explanations beyond exposure to text alone. Therefore, in a recent study (Smeets & Bus, 2012a), we tested the effectiveness of hotspots that trigger a definition of a difficult word when clicked in comparison with a read-only control condition. For instance, in Bear Is in Love With Butterfly (van Haeringen, 2004), a definition of the word shy was audio recorded and linked to a hotspot in the illustration. When the mouse touches Bear's 182 Smeets & Bus red head, a green circle appears around Bear's head, which means that the child has found a hotspot. After clicking on the hotspot, the voiceover explains Bear's mindset: "Bear is shy, his cheeks have turned red." These hotspots enhanced growth in vocabulary beyond encounters with words in text (e.g., Biemiller & Boote, 2006; Blewitt et al., 2009). Hotspots were especially beneficial when words were somewhat familiar and children succeeded at the pretest to select a picture that reflects the target word. That is, when words were receptively familiar prior to the intervention, hotspots improved expressive word knowledge (i.e., saying the word and using it in a sentence). We did not find evidence for (expressive) word learning when the words were completely unknown at pretest.
Questions
Children are more actively involved when they are prompted to answer
questions. Segers and Verhoeven (2002; 2003), for instance, let children
work independently with e-books that included vocabulary games.
Children could be asked to place a cup of coffee on the kitchen table, to
answer a question (such as, Can a carrot be bought in a bakery?), or to
point out or color a certain object in the illustration.
Multiple-choice questions were presented in the e-books focused on in our study (Smeets & Bus, 2012a). For instance, when Bear in the story Bear Is in Love With Butterfly (van Haeringen, 2004) is fanning the fire, the story is interrupted for a question by the computer assistant: "Bear is fanning the fire. In which picture can you see that?" To answer the question, children can click on one of three pictures that appear on screen (the correct image among two distracters). The assistant gives feedback regarding the correctness of the response and provides clues in the cases of incorrect responses. We compared a questioning approach with hotspots that provide definitions after clicking on the hotspot. Our findings provide evidence for the assumption that effects are superior for multiple-choice questions that encourage children to make—rather than take—meaning (Moreno & Valdez, 2005). Even when words were completely unfamiliar prior to listening to the story, questions promoted growth in word knowledge.
Highlighting Print
Many enhanced e-books are furnished with tools of digital guidance
that may focus children's attention on print. When printed text is
available, it might change while it is read by either turning blue (e.g., P.B.
Bear's Birthday Party
[Davis, 1996]), highlighting, increasing in size, or
Picture Storybooks Go Digital: Pros and Cons 183 underlining. In some multimedia e-books, the iconic modes of words are visible in combination with the written word. In P.B. Bear's Birthday Party, for instance, a small-sized picture replaces the printed form of objects (e.g., bed, orange juice, and marmalade). The printed word appears after a click on the small-sized picture, simultaneously with a short animation and pronunciation of the word. Most animations represent the meaning of the noun. For example, the drum starts to move and sounds like a drum. From an experiment with P.B. Bear's Birthday Party, it appeared that this multimedia feature may stimulate interaction with text, resulting in internalization of orthographic features of words (de Jong & Bus, 2002).
A feature that is present in many e-books is a "karaoke-bar" that emphasizes words in synchrony with the reader's voice. For instance, in Disney's storybook apps Cars 2 and Pooh's Birthday Surprise, words are highlighted when they are read aloud. In an eye-tracking study, de Jong and Bus (2012) have demonstrated that highlighted text attracts children's visual attention tremendously and more so when children are more advanced in print knowledge. More importantly, such features have been demonstrated to improve print knowledge such as word recognition (de Jong & Bus, 2012; Korat, 2010) and letter-reading skills (Gong & Levy, 2009).
Besides a reading of the whole text, stories often include options to hear individual words read aloud, either entirely or in segments, which is designed to increase children's awareness of the relation between letters and sounds. For instance, touching words in a storybook app activates a read-aloud of that particular word. For beginning or struggling readers, whole-word reading software can lead to significant gains in written-word recognition, written-word naming, and phonological awareness (e.g., de Jong & Bus, 2002) and can even be equally effective as one-to-one tutoring with an adult in terms of phonological attainment. Especially considering that the most frequent cause of reading difficulties is weaknesses in phonological decoding skills, text-to-speech options can be valuable additions in the earliest years of reading education.
From Research to Practice
A nonstarter of a story cannot wow as an app, no matter how fabulous its
bells and whistles are (Smith, 2011). However, the firm belief that children
benefit most from sharing traditional print books with an adult has yet
to be proved, as there is growing evidence to suggest that some of the
promising features of optimally designed e-books may even outperform
traditional shared print book reading.
184 Smeets & Bus So far, the search for interactive, multimedia features to enrich "good" storybooks has revealed the following: • Animated pictures that include motion and zoomshots provide more clues from which children can extract meanings of difficult words than static pictures, especially when animations highlight elements from the oral text and nonverbal elements are coordinated with verbal elements in text.
• Effects of visual and audio features in enhanced e-books are not yet unraveled, with the exception of SLI children, in which the music and sounds (though not the animated pictures) may interfere with learning.
• Choices between iconic features and text reading have the disadvantage that children mostly prefer the animations and iconic elements at the expense of listening to the book's story text.
• Interactive features like hotspots and questions targeting complex words in the story text support growth in vocabulary more than listening to text alone.
• When children have become active participants in the interaction with the e-reader device by answering multiple-choice questions, interactivity is particularly beneficial (word learning improved by almost 20%).
While 4- and 5-year-old children might dislike print book reading sessions when they are frequently interrupted for questions, questions within interactive e-books may increase children's motivation (Kamil, Intrator, & Kim, 2000). Parents might not know which words to comment on or which events to discuss. E-books, on the other hand, are often designed in collaboration with educators, which results in age-appropriate and more challenging interactions.
However, without a model of book reading routines built up by prior encounters with books read to children by adults, interaction with electronic books might not be possible. In the early stages of book reading, children enjoy interaction with adults more than the book; they need adults to narrow the gap between their everyday world and the world of the book, and caregivers typically read the pictorial content of an illustrated story and its accompanying text more idiosyncratically to younger or less experienced children than to older or more experienced ones (Bus & de Jong, 2006). Also because it is fun and supportive of the emotional bonding with the child, it is important that parents and preschool teachers continue reading to younger age groups.
In sum, this chapter has described that e-books can provide benefits over printed books. However, a lot depends on the quality of e-books, as Picture Storybooks Go Digital: Pros and Cons 185 cutting-edge research in this area has revealed. It is parents' and teachers' responsibility to guide children in their activities with e-books and on the Internet, because children's own choices might not be the ones that should be preferred in a learning context. Young children show an almost universal preference for the exciting commercial programs that fail necessary qualities (Bus & Neuman, 2009).
Many teachers are not that comfortable with technology and fail the expectation and disposition that they can use technology for their own purposes. In the Netherlands, teachers rarely incorporate computer stories into their curriculum or encourage targeted viewing of television stories as an at-home activity. They qualify e-books rather as edutainment than serious learning materials. However, our findings concerning the effects of electronic stories on emergent literacy skills suggest that alternative ways of encountering stories may be a profitable addition to adult-led book reading at home and in kindergarten classrooms. Electronic stories are useful in allowing children to engage in independent reading before they are capable of reading conventional printed texts on their own.
• Illustrations in picture storybooks are works of art that must be interpreted (Schickedanz & Collins, 2012). Think of ways in which technology may help with this.
• Try out an interactive e-book for yourself. Experiment with the interactive features and think about how these features affect your reading of the text. (For a demo of interactive videobooks with questions posed by a built-in tutor, you can visit the bereslim website at web.bereslim.nl/bereslim/bereslimme-boeken/demo-bereslimme-boeken.html.) 1. After reading this chapter, are you convinced that in the age of the CCSS, electronic storybooks can contribute to a positive experience with literacy development that lasts a lifetime? 2. The authors note that e-books are designed for children to "read" independently and that e-books' designs do not match the experiences of shared adult–child reading. Discuss the role of parents and teachers in young children's e-book reading activities.
186 Smeets & Bus R ef eR ences
Barr, R., Zack, E., Garcia, A., & Muentener, P.
Quarterly, 39(4), 378–393. doi: 10.1598/ (2008). Infants' attention and responsiveness to television increases with prior exposure de Jong, M.T., & Bus, A.G. (2012). AVI gaat and parental interaction. Infancy, 13(1), digitaal: oefenboekjes voor beginnende lezers op tablet [Practice material for Biemiller, A., & Boote, C. (2006). An beginning readers on tablet]. Leiden, effective method for building meaning Netherlands: Leiden University/ vocabulary in primary grades. Journal Association of Reading.
of Educational Psychology, 98(1), 44–62. Dickinson, D.K., & Smith, M.W. (1994). Long-term effects of preschool teachers' Blewitt, P., Rump, K.M., Shealy, S.E., & Cook, book readings on low-income children's S.A. (2009). Shared book reading: When vocabulary and story comprehension. and how questions affect young children's Reading Research Quarterly, 29(2), 104–122. word learning. Journal of Educational Psychology, 101(2), 294–304. doi:10.1037/ Elley, W.B. (1989). Vocabulary acquisition from listening to stories. Reading Research Bus, A.G., & de Jong, M.T. (2006). Book Quarterly, 24(2), 174–186. doi:10.2307/747863 sharing: A developmentally appropriate Evans, M.A., & Saint-Aubin, J. (2005). way to foster pre-academic growth. In What children are looking at during S. Rosenkoetter & J. Knapp-Philo (Eds.), shared storybook reading: Evidence Learning to read the world: Language and from eye movement monitoring. literacy in the first three years (pp. 123–144). Psychological Science, 16(11), 913–920. Washington, DC: ZERO TO THREE.
Bus, A.G., & Neuman, S.B. (2009). Afterword. Gong, Z., & Levy, B.A. (2009). Four year old In A.G. Bus & S.B. Neuman (Eds.), children's acquisition of print knowledge Multimedia and literacy development: during electronic storybook reading. Improving achievement for young learners Reading and Writing, 22(8), 889–905. (pp. 273–278). New York: Routledge.
Justice, L.M., & Ezell, H.K. (2000). Enhancing Chiong, C., Ree, J., Takeuchi, L., & Erickson, I. children's print and word awareness (2012). Print books vs. e-books: Comparing through home-based parent intervention. parent-child co-reading on print, basic and American Journal of Speech-Language enhanced e-book platforms. New York: Pathology, 9(3), 257–269.
The Joan Ganz Cooney Center at Sesame Kamil, M.L., Intrator, S.M., & Kim, H.S. (2000). The effects of other technologies Crawley, A.M., Anderson, D.R., Wilder, A., on literacy and literacy learning. In M.L. Williams, M., & Santomero, A. (1999). Kamil, P.B. Mosenthal, P.D. Pearson, & R. Effects of repeated exposures to a single Barr (Eds.), Handbook of reading research episode of the television program Blue's (Vol. 3, pp. 771–778). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Clues on the viewing behaviors and Korat, O. (2010). Reading electronic books comprehension of preschool children. as a support for vocabulary, story Journal of Educational Psychology, 91(4), comprehension and word reading in kindergarten and first grade. Computers & de Jong, M.T., & Bus, A.G. (2002). Quality of Education, 55(1), 24–31. doi:10.1016/ book-reading matters for emergent readers: An experiment with the same book in a Korat, O., & Shamir, A. (2004). Do Hebrew regular or electronic format. Journal of electronic books differ from Dutch Educational Psychology, 94(1), 145–155. electronic books? A replication of a Dutch content analysis. Journal of Computer de Jong, M.T., & Bus, A.G. (2003). How Assisted Learning, 20(4), 257–268. well suited are electronic books to supporting literacy? Journal of Early Labbo, L.D., & Kuhn, M.R. (2000). Weaving Childhood Literacy, 3(2), 147–164. chains of affect and cognition: A young child's understanding of CD-ROM talking de Jong, M.T., & Bus, A.G. (2004). The books. Journal of Literacy Research, 32(2), efficacy of electronic books in fostering kindergarten children's emergent story Mol, S.E., & Bus, A.G. (2011). To read or understanding. Reading Research not to read: A meta-analysis of print Picture Storybooks Go Digital: Pros and Cons 187 exposure from infancy to early adulthood. Sénéchal, M., Thomas, E., & Monker, J.A. Psychological Bulletin, 137(2), 267–296. (1995). Individual differences in 4-year- old children's acquisition of vocabulary Moreno, R., & Valdez, A. (2005). Cognitive during storybook reading. Journal of load and learning effects of having Educational Psychology, 87(2), 218–229. students organize pictures and words in multimedia environments: The role Shamir, A., & Korat, O. (2009). The educational of student interactivity and feedback. electronic book as a tool for supporting Educational Technology Research & children's emergent literacy. In A. Bus & Development, 53(3), 35–45. doi:10.1007/ S.B. Neuman (Eds.), Multimedia and literacy development: Improving achievement for National Governors Association Center for young learners (pp. 168–181). New York: Best Practices & Council of Chief State School Officers. (2010). Common Core State Shamir, A., Korat, O., & Shlafer, I. (2011). The Standards for English language arts and effect of activity with e-book on vocabulary literacy in history/social studies, science, and story comprehension: A comparison and technical subjects. Washington, DC: between kindergarteners at risk of learning Authors. Available at www.corestandards disabilities and typically developing kindergarteners. European Journal of Paivio, A. (1986). Mental representations: A Special Needs Education, 26(3), 311–322. dual coding approach. New York: Oxford University Press.
Smeets, D.J.H., & Bus, A.G. (2012a). Ricci, C.M., & Beal, C.R. (2002). The Interactive electronic storybooks for effect of interactive media on kindergartners to promote vocabulary children's story memory. Journal of growth. Journal of Experimental Child Educational Psychology, 94(1), 138–144. Psychology, 112(1), 36–55. doi:10.1016/ Roskos, K., Brueck, J., & Widman, S. (2009). Smeets, D.J.H., & Bus, A.G. (2012b). The Investigating analytic tools for e-book interactive video book as a word learning device for kindergartners. Under review.
design in early literacy learning. Journal of Smeets, D.J.H., van Dijken, M.J., & Bus, A.G. Interactive Online Learning, 8(3), 218–240.
(2012). Using electronic storybooks to Schickedanz, J.A., & Collins, M.F. (2012). For support word learning in children with young children, pictures in storybooks severe language impairments. Journal are rarely worth a thousand words. of Learning Disabilities, 00(0), 000–000. The Reading Teacher, 65(8), 539–549. Smith, V. (2011, December 18). What makes Schnotz, W., & Rasch, T. (2005). Enabling, a great app? Kirkus Reviews Children's facilitating, and inhibiting effects of (blog). Retrieved December 3, 2012, from animations in multimedia learning: Why reduction of cognitive load can have what-makes-great-app/ negative results on learning. Educational Verhallen, M.J.A.J., & Bus, A.G. (2010). Technology Research and Development, Low-income immigrant pupils learning vocabulary through digital picture Segers, E., & Verhoeven, L. (2002). Multimedia storybooks. Journal of Educational support of early literacy learning. Psychology, 102(1), 54–61. doi:10.1037/ Computers & Education, 39(3), 207–221. Verhallen, M.J.A.J., & Bus, A.G. (2011). Young Segers, E., & Verhoeven, L. (2003). Effects second language learners' visual attention of vocabulary training by computer to illustrations in storybooks. Journal of in kindergarten. Journal of Computer Early Childhood Literacy, 11(4), 480–500. Assisted Learning, 19(4), 557–566. Verhallen, M.J.A.J., Bus, A.G., & de Jong, M.T. Sénéchal, M. (1997). The differential (2006). The promise of multimedia stories effect of storybook reading on for kindergarten children at risk. Journal preschoolers'acquisition of expressive of Educational Psychology, 98(2), 410–419. and receptive vocabulary. Journal of Child Language, 24(1), 123–138. doi:10.1017/ Whitehurst, G.J., & Lonigan, C.J. (2001). Emergent literacy: Development from 188 Smeets & Bus prereaders to readers. In S.B. Neuman & pre-kindergarten-to-grade-5 students' D.K. Dickinson (Eds.), Handbook of early literacy and language outcomes: A lteracy research (Vol. 1, pp. 11–29). New research synthesis. Journal of Educational York: Guilford.
Computing Research, 40(1), 47–87. Zucker, T.A., Moody, A.K., & McKenna, M.C. (2009). The effects of electronic books on L i t eR at u R e a n d M u Lt i M edi a ci t ed
Davis, L. (1996). P.B. is jarig [P.B. Bear's
Janosch. (1998b). ‘Ik maak je weer beter,' zei birthday party] [CD-ROM]. Nieuwegein, Beer [‘I'll make you well again,' said the Netherlands: Bombilla/VNU Interactive Bear] [CD-ROM]. Baarn, Netherlands: Het Spectrum Electronic Publishing.
de Wijs, I. (2001). Rokko krokodil [Rokko Thomas, V., & Gorky, P. (1996). Heksenspul met the crocodile]. Rotterdam, Netherlands: Hennie de Heks en de Kat Helmer [Winnie the Witch] [CD-ROM]. Nieuwegein, Janosch. (1998a). Groot feest voor Tijger [Big party for Tiger] [CD-ROM]. Baarn, van Haeringen, A. (2004). Beer is op Vlinder Netherlands: Het Spectrum Electronic [Bear is in love with butterfly]. Amsterdam: Picture Storybooks Go Digital: Pros and Cons 189

Source: http://digilitey.eu/wp-content/uploads/2015/09/Picture-Storybooks-Go-Digital.pdf

Print

Internal Medicine Journal 44 (2014) Australian Centre for Sexual Health, Sydney, New South Wales, Australia Key words erectile dysfunction, phosphodiesterase type 5 inhibitor, alprostadil, intrapenile prosthesis, In the past 30 years, advances in basic science have been instrumental in the evolution vacuum constriction device. of the male sexual health treatment paradigm from a psychosexual model to a newmodel, which includes oral and intracavernosal injection pharmacotherapy, vacuum

Microsoft word - sbfogel comments to pres council bioethics.doc

2639 S. La Cienega Blvd Los Angeles, CA 90034 (310) 204-6010 Fax (310) 204-0891 President's Council on Bioethics September 12, 2008 Arlington, Virginia Testimony of Susan Berke Fogel, J.D. National Health Law Program Good morning. Thank you Dr. Pelegrino and Council members for the opportunity to offer comment this morning on the impact of refusal clauses and institutional restrictions in the delivery of health care services. I am Susan Berke Fogel, J.D. I am an attorney with the National Health Law Program, a national public interest law firm that seeks to improve health care for America's working and unemployed poor, minorities, the elderly and people with disabilities. NHeLP serves legal services programs, community-based organizations, policymakers, the private bar, providers and individuals who work to preserve a health care safety net for the millions of uninsured or underinsured low-income people. We approach refusal clauses and denials of care from this quality and access perspective. We also understand that low-income women and other marginalized populations experience greater health disparities that are exacerbated by restrictions on care. The basic principles of modern health care delivery are evidence-based practice, patient centeredness, and prevention, collectively ensuring quality care. Failure to adhere to prevailing "standards of care" harms the individual patient in real and concrete ways, undermines the medical system, and jeopardizes the health of the general public. These principles may be compromised by a range of structural factors such as lack of insurance, restricted geographic access, cost, language barriers, and immigration status. We are hoping that current political movements will be successful in promoting strategies to address these structural barriers to quality of care. At the same time, health care refusals and denials of care are proliferating in the U.S. based on personal religious and moral beliefs that have nothing to do with scientific evidence, good medical practice or the needs of the patient. These restrictions and denials of care, which most often are related to reproductive health and disproportionately affect women, should be scrutinized to assess their impact on quality health care and redressed when they fall below the standard of care. Analyses of health care denials traditionally construct the issue as a conflict of rights within the provider-patient relationship: the health care provider's right to exercise individual conscience

Copyright © 2008-2016 No Medical Care